Bulletin of Monetary Economics and Banking, Vol. 21, No. 2 (2018), pp. 141 - 148
CULTURAL HERITAGE AND INNOVATION
James B. Ang1
1Department of Economics, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.
ABSTRACT
Cultural heritage is a major driver of behavioral, social, and economic norms in a society. This paper studies the relationship between culture and economic development by focusing on how individualism is related to technological innovation. It hypothesizes that individualistic people tend to have beliefs and views that emphasize the importance of innovation and creativity. Using
Keywords: Technology; Innovation; Individualism.
JEL Classification: O30; Z10.
Article history: |
|
Received |
: June 5, 2018 |
Revised |
: October 11, 2018 |
Accepted |
: October 19, 2018 |
Available online |
: October 31, 2018 |
https://doi.org/10.21098/bemp.v21i1.961
142Bulletin of Monetary Economics and Banking, Volume 21, Number 2, October 2018
I. INTRODUCTION
The idea that culture affects economic development can be traced back to the work of Weber (1930), who was among the first to articulate the relevance of an individualistic culture for
In particular, the congruence between economic growth, innovation and technology adoption in individualistic societies has received much attention in the literature (Ball, 2001; Giuliano et al., 2006; Ashraf and Galor, 2007; Tabellini, 2008; Gorodnichenko and Roland, 2017). Among others, Triandis (1995, 2001) argues that individualism is the most significant driver of cultural disparity across countries that also has a significant influence on technology adoption. This paper is related to this strand of literature by establishing the relationship between innovation and individualism.
An individualistic society fosters values and societal norms where personal freedom and achievements are more highly emphasized than collective actions and interests. Hofstede (1980) defines individualism as “a preference for a loosely- knit social framework in which individuals are expected to take care of only themselves and their immediate families”. Studies by
Based on these tenets, we hypothesize that individualistic societies tend to foster and encourage a psychological inclination towards innovation. The paper proceeds as follows. Section II provides an empirical investigation of our hypothesis. Section III concludes.
II. EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS
The following model is regressed to investigate the relationship between individualism and innovation:
(1)
where Innovation captures an individual’s inclination towards innovation, IDV is the degree of individualism, cv’ is a vector of control variables, as described below, and ε is an unobserved error term.
Cultural Heritage and Innovation |
143 |
|
|
We investigate the relationship between innovation and individualistic culture using
(b)progress toward a less impersonal and more humane society; (c) progress toward a society in which ideas count more than money; and (d) the fight against crime. A value of 1 is assigned if the respondents choose (c) and 0 if they choose other answers. This variable is labelled as value of ideas in this study. The last question asks “are the tasks you perform at work mostly routine tasks or mostly creative tasks?” The answers can range from “mostly routine tasks” (1) to “mostly not routine tasks” (10). We divide the raw scores by 10. The resulting variable captures the extent to which the tasks performed are creative, and hence we call this measure creative tasks.
We measure individualism by referring to three WVS variables capturing the respondents’ beliefs and views on the importance of tradition, the justification of divorce, and the significance of family in an individual’s life. Schwartz (1992, 1994) argues that collectivism is associated with tradition and conformity at the individual level. Individuals who value tradition seek to preserve the customary ways of doing things, and any changes make them uncomfortable. Individuals who value conformity abide by clear rules and structures, and prefer to do what they are told. Accordingly, we use relevant data from WVS to capture the
Next, Inglehart (1990) and Triandis (1995) argue that individualism emphasizes the pursuit of one’s
(1)to “always justifiable” (10). We divide this variable by 10 and call it divorce justification.
Finally, Alesina and Giuliano (2010) use individual responses from the
WVS regarding the role of the family to measure the power of family ties. They emphasize the role of the strength of family ties in influencing various economic
144Bulletin of Monetary Economics and Banking, Volume 21, Number 2, October 2018
outcomes. In particular, individuals who view their family as important in their life develop strong family ties, and this belief fosters the shaping of societies that are characterized by traditional family structures where the male is the bread winner whilst the female is dedicated to housework. This argument, along with those of Inglehart (1990) and Triandis (1995) above, suggests that societies with weak family ties tend to be more individualistic.
Accordingly, the third question assesses how important the family is in a person’s life. It can take values from 1 to 4 with 1 being “very important” and 4 “not at all important”. We assign a value of 0 to answers which indicate that family is “very important” or “rather important”, 0.5 to “not very important” and 1 to “not at all important”. This variable is labelled as family unimportance for the purpose of our study.
All regressions include age, age squared, marital status, gender, educational attainment (primary, secondary or tertiary), and income (low, middle or high) as control variables. Country fixed effects are included throughout. In addition, robust standard errors clustered by region are employed. Sources and description of all variables used in the estimations are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Sources and Description of Variables
This table describes the variables and their sources. Column 1 notes the name of the variable, column 2 has descriptions while the final column contains the sources of data.
Variable |
Description |
Source |
|
[A] Proxies for Individualism |
|
A proxy for individualism based on WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
|
|
(V79) “Please listen to each description and tell me how much |
|
|
each person is or is not like you: Tradition is important to this |
|
|
person.” The answers range from “(1) very much like me” to |
|
|
“(6) Not at all like me”. The raw scores are divided by 6 to |
|
|
construct the variable. |
|
Divorce |
A proxy for individualism based on the WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
Justification |
(V205) “Justifiable: Divorce”. The answers range from “(1) |
|
|
Never justifiable” to “(10) Always justifiable”. The raw scores |
|
|
are divided by 10 to construct the variable. |
|
Family |
A proxy for individualism based on the WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
Unimportance |
(V4) “Importance in life: Family”. The answers range from |
|
|
“(1) Very important” to “(4) Not at all important”. A value of |
|
|
0 is assigned to answers which indicate that family is “very |
|
|
important” or “rather important”, 0.5 to “not very important” |
|
|
and 1 to “not at all important”. |
|
|
[B] Proxies for Innovation |
|
Credibility of |
A proxy for innovation based on the WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
Science |
(V153) “Whenever science and religion conflict, religion is |
|
|
always right”. The answers range from “(1) Strongly agree” |
|
|
to “(4) Strongly disagree”. A value of 0.5 is assigned if the |
|
|
respondents disagree and 1 if they strongly disagree. If they |
|
|
agree or strongly agree the values is 0. |
|
Cultural Heritage and Innovation |
145 |
|
|
Table 1.
Sources and Description of Variables (Continued)
This table describes the variables and their sources. Column 1 notes the name of the variable, column 2 has descriptions while the final column contains the sources of data.
Variable |
Description |
Source |
Value of Ideas |
A proxy for innovation based on the WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
|
(V64) “Most important (First choice)”. The answers offer |
|
|
the following options “(1) a stable economy”; “(2) progress |
|
|
toward a less impersonal and more humane society”; “(3) |
|
|
progress toward a society in which ideas count more than |
|
|
money”; and “(4) the fight against crime”. A value of 1 is |
|
|
assigned if the respondents choose (3) and 0 if they choose |
|
|
other answers. |
|
Creative Tasks |
A proxy for innovation based on the WVS survey question |
WVS Database (2014) |
|
(V232) “Nature of tasks: routine vs. creative”. The answers |
|
|
range from “(1) mostly routine tasks” to “(10) mostly not |
|
|
routine tasks”. The raw scores are divided by 10 to construct |
|
|
the variable. |
|
|
[C] Control Variables |
|
Age |
The respondent’s age. |
WVS Database (2014) |
Marital Status |
The marital status of the respondent. |
WVS Database (2014) |
Gender |
The gender of the respondent. |
WVS Database (2014) |
Educational |
The respondent’s highest level of education attained. |
WVS Database (2014) |
Attainment |
|
|
Income |
The respondent’s income level. |
WVS Database (2014) |
Table 2 reports the estimation results using an OLS estimator. Consistent with our predictions, the results in columns (1) to (3) indicate that
Table 2.
OLS
This table reports regression results. The standard errors are clustered at the region level. The coefficients are standardized beta coefficients. Symbols *, ** and *** indicate significance levels at the 10%, 5% and 1%, respectively. All regressions include age, age squared, marital status, gender, educational attainment (primary, secondary or tertiary), and income (low, middle or high) as control variables. An intercept and country fixed effects are included throughout.
Dependent |
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
(9) |
|
Credibility |
Credibility |
Credibility |
Value of |
Value of |
Value of |
Creative |
Creative |
Creative |
||
Variable is: |
||||||||||
of Science |
of Science |
of Science |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Tasks |
Tasks |
Tasks |
||
|
||||||||||
0.11*** |
|
|
0.02*** |
|
|
0.03*** |
|
|
||
|
(13.34) |
|
|
(3.29) |
|
|
(3.92) |
|
|
|
Divorce Justification |
|
0.11*** |
|
|
0.02*** |
|
|
0.02*** |
|
|
|
|
(11.72) |
|
|
(4.01) |
|
|
(3.71) |
|
146Bulletin of Monetary Economics and Banking, Volume 21, Number 2, October 2018
Table 2.
OLS
(Continued)
This table reports regression results. The standard errors are clustered at the region level. The coefficients are standardized beta coefficients. Symbols *, ** and *** indicate significance levels at the 10%, 5% and 1%, respectively. All regressions include age, age squared, marital status, gender, educational attainment (primary, secondary or tertiary), and income (low, middle or high) as control variables. An intercept and country fixed effects are included throughout.
Dependent |
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
(9) |
|
Credibility |
Credibility |
Credibility |
Value of |
Value of |
Value of |
Creative |
Creative |
Creative |
||
Variable is: |
||||||||||
of Science |
of Science |
of Science |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Tasks |
Tasks |
Tasks |
||
|
||||||||||
Family |
|
|
0.03*** |
|
|
0.01*** |
|
|
0.01 |
|
Unimportance |
|
|
(4.64) |
|
|
(3.01) |
|
|
(0.03) |
|
0.382 |
0.382 |
0.376 |
0.019 |
0.019 |
0.019 |
0.124 |
0.123 |
0.123 |
||
Observations |
70490 |
69992 |
71043 |
74923 |
74220 |
75657 |
62564 |
62045 |
63224 |
|
No. of Countries |
57 |
59 |
59 |
56 |
56 |
56 |
57 |
57 |
57 |
|
No. of Districts |
749 |
749 |
750 |
744 |
743 |
744 |
750 |
749 |
750 |
|
Controls |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
|
Country Dummies |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Next, we provide evidence based on analyses that consider only data of the U.S. Using the U.S. data serves two important purposes. First, the U.S. is not only the most individualistic country in our sample, but also the world technological frontier in many respects. It is therefore crucial to understand whether individualism has the potential to explain preferences for innovation across individuals in one of the most innovative and individualistic countries in the world. Second, using data only for the U.S. enables us to control for a range of factors such as institutions, geography, language, and history that vary dramatically across countries. The resulting estimates, which are less affected by these factors, may provide useful evidence to complement the estimates based on full sample.
Table 3.
OLS
(United States Only)
This table reports regression results. The standard errors are clustered at the region level. The coefficients are standardized beta coefficients. Symbols *, ** and *** indicate significance levels at the 10%, 5% and 1%, respectively. All regressions include age, age squared, marital status, gender, educational attainment (primary, secondary or tertiary), and income (low, middle or high) as control variables. An intercept and country fixed effects are included throughout.
Dependent |
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
(9) |
|
Credibility |
Credibility |
Credibility |
Value of |
Value of |
Value of |
Creative |
Creative |
Creative |
||
Variable is: |
||||||||||
of Science |
of Science |
of Science |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Ideas |
Tasks |
Tasks |
Tasks |
||
|
||||||||||
0.27*** |
|
|
0.01 |
|
|
0.06*** |
|
|
||
Divorce Justification |
(35.39) |
0.25*** |
|
(1.61) |
0.01 |
|
(7.89) |
0.07*** |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
Family Unimportance |
|
(31.04) |
0.04*** |
|
(1.58) |
0.01 |
|
(8.15) |
(0.01) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
|
|
|
(5.26) |
|
|
(0.75) |
|
|
||
0.102 |
0.092 |
0.033 |
0.007 |
0.007 |
0.007 |
0.133 |
0.132 |
0.129 |
||
Observations |
14973 |
14828 |
15091 |
16627 |
16452 |
16838 |
15333 |
15186 |
15507 |
|
Controls |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Yes |
Cultural Heritage and Innovation |
147 |
|
|
The results reported in Table 3 indicate that in five out of nine cases we find evidence supporting the notion that individualistic people tend to value the importance of innovation more. In cases where the coefficients for the proxies of individualism are statistically significant, their sizes are larger than those found in Table 1, which uses data for all countries. These findings highlight that individualism matters for the perception regarding the importance of innovation and creativity, even within a country that has achieved a very high level of technological sophistication.
III. CONCLUSION
Technology and innovation play a pivotal role in the economic growth of a country. Scientific advancement can account for the differences in incomes across countries (Hall and Jones, 1999; Mokyr, 2005; Aghion and Howitt, 2007; Comin et al., 2008; Comin and Hobijn, 2010). A major policy agenda for all nations in the world, therefore, is to promote the advancement of scientific inventions and innovation. It is a priority for most governments to create and foster an environment that will help bolster technological growth. Understanding the cultural dynamics that affect innovation is potentially useful for creating an environment conducive to scientific innovation.
This paper studies the relationship between individualism and innovation. An
REFERENCES
Aghion, P., & Howitt, P. (2007). Capital, Innovation, and Growth Accounting. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 23,
Alesina, A., & Giuliano, P. (2010). The Power of the Family. Journal of Economic Growth, 15,
Ashraf, Q., & Galor, O. (2007). Cultural Assimilation, Cultural Diffusion, and the Origin of the Wealth of Nations. CEPR Working Paper No 6444.
Ball, R. (2001). Individualism, Collectivism, and Economic Development. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 573,
Comin, D., & Hobijn, B. (2010). ‘An Exploration of Technology Diffusion’. American Economic Review, 100,
Comin, D., Hobijn, B., & Rovito, E. (2008). Technology Usage Lags. Journal of Economic Growth, 13,
Dion, K. K., & Dion, K. L. (1996). Cultural Perspectives on Romantic Love. Personal Relationships, 3,
Giuliano, P., Spilimbergo, A., & Tonon, G. (2006). Genetic, Cultural, and Geographical Distances. IZA Discussion Papers, 2229.
148Bulletin of Monetary Economics and Banking, Volume 21, Number 2, October 2018
Gorodnichenko, Y., & Roland, G. (2011). Individualism, Innovation, and Long- Run Growth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 108,
_____ (2017). Culture, Institutions, and the Wealth of Nations. Review of Economics
and Statistics, 99,
Greif, A. (1994). Cultural Beliefs and the Organization of Society: A Historical and Theoretical Reflection on Collectivist and Individualist Societies. Journal of Political Economy, 102,
Hall, R. E., & Jones, C. I. (1999). Why Do Some Countries Produce So Much More Output Per Worker Than Others? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114,
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in
Inglehart, R. (1990). Culture Shift in Advanced Industrial Society. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Lal, D. (1999). Unintended Consequences: The Impact of Factor Endowments, Culture, and Politics on
Lester, D. (1995). Individualism and Divorce. Psychological Reports, 76, 258. Macfarlane, A. (1979). The Origins of English Individualism. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Mokyr, J. (2005).
_____ (2014). Culture, Institutions, and Modern Growth. Institutions, Property Rights,
and Economic Growth: The Legacy of Douglass North eds. S. Galiani and I.
Sened. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed. M. P. Zanna. San Diego, CA: Academic Press,
_____ (1994). Beyond Individualism and Collectivism: New Cultural Dimensions of
Values. Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications eds. U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi,
Tabellini, G. (2008). Presidential Address: Institutions and Culture. Journal of the European Economic Association, 6,
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.
_____ (2001).
Social Psychology, 69,
Weber, M. (1930). The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit of Capitalism. London: Routledge. WVS Database. (2014). World Value Survey Wave 6
Association (www.worldvaluessurvey.org). Madrid, Spain.